The dissociative identification disorder identification controversy

Dissociative Individuality Disorder (DID), formerly known as Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD), has been more popular and studied over the years. Although Dissociative Identity Disorder was officially accepted by the DSM-IV as a valid psychiatric analysis, intense question about its validity continues to be common. You will find two basic positions that dominate the controversy of DID. There are some who assume that this can be a valid identification. Proponents of DID argue that those who avoid to detect their patient's with DID and neglect to discover the disorder are not well trained. While others argue that DID is not really a valid psychiatric diagnosis. These people argue that DID is induced by misguided people who look continuously looks for the right therapist until they have the medical diagnosis that they wanted. Moreover, these people assume that DID can be an iatrogenic phenomenon brought on by incompetent therapists through hypnotherapy and suggestions.

For those who claim that DID is a valid medical diagnosis they point out that the identification of Dissociative Identification Disorder is extremely complex. Among the reasons that it is complex to identify is due to how difficult it is to differentiate from many other syndromes. Moreover, DID may also coexist with more familiar and less questionable syndromes. In 1984, Coons stated that DID can be lost with other dissociative disorders such as psychogenic amnesia and fugue, and depersonalization disorder. Furthermore, DID may also be perplexed with atypical dissociative disorder experience by those who had been in prisoner or hostage situations and dissociates from the strain such as physical and emotional misuse that they put up with brought on by their captors. Since phobias, ambiance swings and transformation reactions like pseudo seizures, paralysis, and blindness are common with nervousness, affective, and somatoform disorder they may also co-exist with multiple personality disorder. In order to obtain more facts that a patient has DID, Coons advised that information from exterior options such as family members, friends, coworkers, and hospital staff is important. When asking these people, a clinician should ask about evidence of personality changes, consistent laying, use of third person, handwriting changes, and many more subtle indicators that might provide proof DID (Coons, 1984). In addition to Coons, in a study conducted by Horevitz and Braun (1984), they discovered that DID can co-exist with borderline personality. They studied 93 patients with "confirmed" diagnoses of DID. During the study they were only to evaluate 33 of the 93 circumstances. They discovered that 23 of the 33 or 70% of their sample also qualified for the medical diagnosis of borderline personality (Horevitz & Braun 1984). Similar to the studies conducted by Horevitz and Braun, and Coons, Clary, Burstin, and Carpenter figured DID has a lot in keeping with borderline personality. They drew their conclusions from 11 patients who have been poor and referred through public businesses and women's shelter. On the review, Clary et al. noted the difference between their studies and Richard Kluft's (1982) findings. Kluft found borderline characteristics in only 22. 8% of his 70 subjects. 45% of them were described as "neurotic mixtures" and 32% were referred to as "hysterical-depressive. " Clary et al. assumed that their results were not the same as Kluft because Kluft's patients were from a private psychoanalytic practice. Because of this, their functioning is way better as a result of demands rigorous psychoanalytic psychotherapy requires (Clary, Burstin, & Carpenter, 1984).

Proponents of DID like Carol North, Jo-Ellyn Ryall, Daniel Ricci, and Richard Wetzel explain documented physiologic variations between personalities of patients with DID. These cases were then recognized by the argument that these symptoms could not be replicated by normal people or professional celebrities. This is because, there are distinctive patterns among the several personalities a patient with DID has. These distinctions can be detected through the "positron emission tomography (PET) scans, evoked potentials, tone of voice prints, aesthetic acuity, eyeball muscle balance, visible field size, galvanic skin response, electroencephalographic habits, electromyography, and cerebral blood flow (North et al. , 1993, pg. 29). "

Proponents argue that DID patients are quiet, unassuming, and shy people who do not seek general population attention. Relating to Kluft (1985), DID patients try to disguise their conditions because they are concerned about the reactions that they are heading to get from unwilling therapists. In various studies conducted by Kluft, he found multiple times the reluctance that DID patients feel when witnessing a therapist. In one of his studies, he found that only 40% of patients with DID showed subtle tips of the disorder while 40% exhibited no overt symptoms at all. For the reason that study he found that the identification of DID was an inverse romance to how clear the symptoms were in the individual. During this review he also found that multiples who go into treatment do because of affective, psychotic-like, or somatoform symptoms instead of traditional DID symptoms. Because the display of the disorder is often simple, Kluft points out that it's very important to clinicians to work very difficult to elicit a history compatible with DID. Comparable to Coons, Kluft specifically brought up that it's very important to clinicians to utilize indirect questions for patients who show the symptoms of DID (Kluft, 1984). In 1986 he found that 50% of DID patients withheld evidence of DID during their first analysis, and 90% said that at one point in their lives they attempted to cover the manifestations of DID. Kluft also discovered that there are some cases where the symptoms of DID aren't voluntarily provided to the therapist because patients are unaware they have the disorder (Kluft, 1986). Regardless of a lack of consensus that DID is a valid psychiatric disorder, proponents of DID, like Kluft, have divided DID into subtypes. Later Kluft (1991) identified the typology of DID presentation that includes the next types: "Classic MPD, latent MPD, posttraumatic MPD, extremely complex of fragmented MPD, Epochal or sequential MPD, isomorphic MPD, coconscious MPD, possessioniform MPD, reincarnation/mediumistic MPD, atypical MPD, hidden knowledge MPD, ostensible imaginary companionship MPD, covert MPD, phenocopy MPD, somatoform MPD, Orphan indication MPD, switch-dominated MPD, random MPD, modular MPD, quasi-roleplaying MPD, and pseudo-false positive MPD (North et. al, 1993, pg. 30). "

Another one who feels that DID is a valid psychiatric disorder is Brad Foote. Foote (1999) wrote a paper that features why DID can certainly be mistaken for hysterical phenomena. Among the key critiques that other people have is that DID does not arise "naturally. " Instead, its symptoms are today's version of "hysteria. " In this particular view, many believe that patients may create or record dissociative symptoms both intentionally and unintentionally to be able to believe the ill role. Opponents believe this sick and tired role is helpful as a result of attention that they get from friends, family, and their therapist. In addition to that, some proponents believe the therapist has a big affect on the patient's pathology and thus contributes to this phenomenon. Regarding to the view, patients did not have any observeable symptoms of DID present prior to finding a therapist. Alternatively, those who treat DID patients claim that: "1. There's a naturally occurring demonstration of DID, prior to therapist ideas; 2. Patients do not accept the DID analysis willingly, and in fact usually battle at least as hard to reject as, for extremely ego-dystonic; 3. DID symptoms do not disappear when ignored; and 4. The disorder actually begins in child years, in the framework of overwhelming stress, and there could not possibly be caused by the adult therapist alongside the patient (Foote, 1999, pg. 321). " Foote describes that for a typical DID patient, powerlessness occurs in a severe level. He says that it's common for a DID patient to truly have a long history of mistreatment, usually including sexual abuse. With regards to diagnosing DID, Foote talks about a situation when a typical DID patient will find herself in. When the therapist's bias that the DID patient is creating her symptoms to seek attention, this bias is only going to be proved by every one of the dilemma and attention that the individual will have. Set up patient will anything dramatic depends upon the therapist. Consequently, if the individual is talking to a skeptical listener, the individual will feel powerless and will cause her to stop, or become hysterical and eager in her communications to be able to explain to her therapist her symptoms. Furthermore, if the therapist has a strong bias that "switching" from one personality to another is feigned, there are no data that could falsify this affirmation. In the event the patient's switch is simple the observer would feel that there is absolutely no big package and it doesn't seem like the patient has a different personality. Likewise, if the individual has a remarkable swap the therapist would believe that her activities are exaggerated and clearly unreal. With this said, however, Foote wanted to clarify that DID examination is not immune system from factitious presentations for the intended purpose of attention-seeking. However, skepticism may become a hurdle to the opportunity of the identification to be identified. To conclude, Foote wanted to point out that first, DID by its characteristics is unavoidably dramatic and that causes clinicians to be unconvinced before they have ever seen a DID patient. Because of this, he induces clinicians to be open-minded and be aware of how complicated DID can be. He thinks that if a clinician requires their time for you to patiently immerse him/herself in the world of DID, they will discover the possibilities of DID that aren't readily available superficially. Second, Foote points out that it's very important to clinicians to, "Hopefully, we can call upon our own interior resources of calmness and assurance that if a tale is true it'll ultimately be been told, and proceed to communicate consequently (pg. 342). "

According to Frank Putnam (1996), A couple of three basic criticism when it comes to the validity of DID. The first one is that DID is an iatrogenic disorder induced by the psychiatrist. Second, critics say that DID is produced by the multimedia. Finally, critics say that DID circumstance numbers are growing exponentially over time. For the first debate on DID being caused by a psychiatrist, Putnam highlights that we now have at least two scientific studies that contain shown that we now have no distinct distinctions between those who find themselves diagnosed with DID and was cured with or without hypnosis. Also, many patients who've never been cared for using hypnotherapy was identified as having DID. This demonstrates the accusation that the misuse of hypnotherapy is in charge of the disorder is not correct. Second, by looking at decade's value of research on the advertising effects on behavior, Putnam says that it is clear that exposure to specific press is not really a sure cause of a certain tendencies. He highlights that the portrayal of violence in the press is more prevalent than the depiction of DID. Yet, critics say that the tiny amount DID portrayals in the multimedia is significantly accountable for the increase in diagnosed cases. Finally regarding DID conditions increasing exponentially; Putnam says that it's common for critics to fill their numbers with no evidence aiding their figures. Corresponding to him, after plotting the numbers of published conditions of DID he discovered that they may have increased but not as dramatic as critics make it sound. In fact, on the same period of time other disorders such as Lyme disease, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and serious fatigue syndrome show the same or faster increase in published cases compared to DID. These results mirror the results of basic progression in the medical field. Disorders increase in published situations may be due to the new discoveries of symptoms that used to be unrelated. As new symptoms are located to be related to certain disorders, a lot more the physicians can identify the condition. Ultimately, Putnam believes that DID meets the standards of "content validity criterion, criterion-related validity, and create validity considered necessary for the validity of your psychiatric analysis (pg. 263). "

One of the controversial subject areas about DID and its diagnosis is the fact hypnosis elicit DID. Richard Kluft, a Clinical of Teacher of Psychiatry in Temple College or university School of Medication believes that hypnotherapy or advice may be the reason some patients have alternate personalities. However, he thinks that iatrogenesis or hypnosis do not clarify DID. While Putnam (1986) did not detect and differences in clinical presentation, symptoms, or past history between patients who have been hypnotized and those who weren't. Furthermore, Ross et al. (1989) conducted a study where they examined 236 patients who had been diagnosed with DID. They discovered that only a 3rd of the patients have been hypnotized prior to being actually identified as having DID. Furthermore analysis, Ross conducted another review where he likened DID patients of psychiatrists who specialize in DID and patients of psychiatrists who didn't focus on this disorder. They concluded that DID is not iatrogenic. The idea of iatrogenesis has been continuously disputed. The analysis that Ross et al. (1989) found persuasive evidence that presents that DID is a genuine disorder with steady key features with convincing evidence.

Nicholas Spanos, a Teacher of Mindset at Ottawa's Carelton College or university conducted two experiments which explores DID. He argues that DID patients are not unaggressive victims. Instead, these are patients who do what to purposefully be diagnosed with the disorder. He also argued that therapists assist these patients achieve their goals. The therapists provide encouragement, information, and validation for different identities. For his first experiment in 1984, he previously forty-eight undergraduate volunteers as his subject matter. These were asked to role-play an accuse multiple murder named Henry or Betty whose legal professional decided to type in a not liable plea. They were told a "psychiatrist" would interview them and might even use hypnotherapy. If hypnotherapy was used these were asked to also role-play being hypnotized. The subjects were not advised anything about DID. There have been three possible conditions that eight men and eight women were randomly assigned to. Within the first condition, the subject matter were asked if the was feeling the same thing as Harry or Betty or if they noticed any different. In the next condition, content were told that that they had complex personalities but hypnotherapy allows the therapist to get behind the "wall" that hid their inner thoughts from understanding. Furthermore, the hypnotist would be able to speak to their other personality under hypnotherapy. In the third, that was the control group, themes were informed that personality was complicated and included walled-off thoughts and thoughts. Spanos also administered a five-item phrase completion and a differential test to all or any subjects (it included all their different roles, in which a second personality was enacted). Following the "psychiatrist" told the things their personality, they asked the same four questions to each subject matter. The replies were scored by judges who didn't know the subject's treatment teams. The results showed that 81% of the content who have been asked if indeed they felt the same thing as harry or Betty or are they different and 31% of themes in hidden-part treatment adopted a new name. 70% of these subjects who followed a new name experienced two different identities. 63% of subjects in the hypnotic treatments displayed spontaneous amnesia. While, none of them of the control subject matter used a different name or acquired amnesia. In his discourse Spanos makes four things. First, only the subject matter who had been hypnotized used another name, reported two different identities, and amnesia. Second, all but one subject who possessed multiple personalities admitted guilt on the next administration. Those that had no multiple personalities continuously refused guilt. Third, Spanos points out how easy it is to artificial multiple personality even without the knowledge of DID. Finally, Spanos points out that multiple typically show contrasting personalities. Spanos feels that the amnesia of his themes was a tactical way to regulate the subject's capacity to recall a memory in response to the situation at hand (Spanos 1984).

Another point that opponents want to make is that DID is well suited for providing patients a way to avoid being accountable for their activities. Kluft (1985) described some DID patients who value their disorder. In medical center wards, other patients complain that DID patients avoid accountability and responsibility. DID may also be accounted for the failures a person with DID wishes to avoid facing. DID patients utilize this disorder as a justification for their troubles or failures to explain why these were in the problem that these were in. Relating to Bliss, another way that DID can be good for others is that it shows an outlet to express actions that are considered unacceptable, such as intimate habits, physical aggressions, or drug abuse. Another personality may misuse chemicals or rape, as the host personality would never do any such thing. This meets the explanations that alternative personalities are usually irresponsible and loves to act out with the coordinator personality as proper. Alternative personalities are also intended to manage unpleasant emotions that the patient wishes to avoid. Specific emotions are assigned to a personality as a way to avoid needing to recognize strong or agonizing emotions. (Kluft, 1985).

Since the truth of Eve Black became famous Thigpen and Cleckley published a newspaper where they revealed matter for the "epidemic" of DID situations. There were a large number of patients who travelled thousands of kilometers to see different therapists until they received the diagnosis that they desired. Not just that, but they proceed through great measures such as communicating on the telephone in several voices, sending images of different "selves, " and writing letters with different handwritings for every paragraph. With regards to these folks, these desperate activities would not stop until these were diagnosed with DID. Another group of patients wrongly diagnoses with DID were attention-seeking hysterics who are afflicted by the labeling process. While, the previous category that they explained are groups of those who aren't content with their self-concept so they use dissociation to allow the unacceptable aspects of their personalities to be indicated (Thigpen & Cleckley, 1984).

Proponents of DID assert that DID is a genuine disorder which has a valid diagnosis, whereas skeptics dispute that DID is an iatrogenic or faked condition. Both of these different quarrels may both be persuasive but neither of these does not answer the question of the validity of DID. It is important to evaluate these arguments to look for the extent of the identification of DID. Current knowledge of the scientific phenomenology of DID cannot be considered as either evidence or disproof that DID is a valid diagnostic entity. Kluft demands "working research rather than fruitless issue (pg. 3). " Future studies on DID will have many opportunities to address the problems that both proponents and competitors of DID medical diagnosis validity pose.

Also We Can Offer!

Other services that we offer

If you don’t see the necessary subject, paper type, or topic in our list of available services and examples, don’t worry! We have a number of other academic disciplines to suit the needs of anyone who visits this website looking for help.

How to ...

We made your life easier with putting together a big number of articles and guidelines on how to plan and write different types of assignments (Essay, Research Paper, Dissertation etc)